Please consider a donation to the Higher Intellect project. See https://preterhuman.net/donate.php or the Donate to Higher Intellect page for more info. |
Group Authorship Among Second Graders Using Multimedia Composing Software
Apple Classrooms of Tomorrow Research
Report Number 14
The Negotiation of Group Authorship Among Second Graders Using Multimedia
Composing Software
Author
Brian Reilly
University of California at Berkeley
Apple Computer, Inc.
1 Infinite Loop
Cupertino, CA 95014
Preface
Begun in 1985, Apple Classrooms of Tomorrow (ACOT)SM is a
research and development collaboration among public schools, universities,
research agencies and Apple Computer, Inc. ACOT explores, develops and demonstrates
the powerful uses of technologies in teaching and learning. In all ACOT
endeavors, instruction and assessment are as integral to learning as technology.
Supporting a constructivist approach to learning, technology is used as
knowledge-building tools. As students collaborate, create media-rich compositions
and use simulations and models, researchers investigate four aspects of
learning: tasks, interactions, situations and tools. The research is formative.
The findings guide ACOT staff and teachers as they refine their approach
to learning, teaching and professional development. ACOT teachers and students
often use the most advanced technologies available, including experimental
technologies, to help us envision the future and improve the educational
process.
ACOT views technology as a necessary and catalytic part of the effort required
to fundamental restructure America's education system. We hope that by sharing
our results with parents, educators, policy makers, and technology developers
the lessons of ACOT will contribute to the advancement of educational reform.
Abstract
Beginning with a review of relevant literature on learning and computers,
this report focuses on a group of five second graders in the process of
creating a multimedia presentation for their class. Using software that
combines images, sound and text, the students took on a variety of production
roles. Each one contributed at least one image and sound element to the
final composition. Each task the students performed--manipulating hardware
or software, choosing images from books or directing other students--was
essential to the overall success of the composition, but the role of director,
taken on by one girl in the group, became the key to its completion.
The success of this episode was facilitated, in part, by the teacher's interpretation
of how the software might be used, and a classroom environment that supported
the kinds of independent and collaborative activities that the software
encouraged.
Introduction
According to Vygotsky (1978), the tools that we use to manipulate signs
-- in the form of language or other symbol systems -- mediate our interactions
with the world and restructure our mental activity. In human societies,
people perform numerous tasks with the aid of tools as simple as hammers
and saws or as complex as writing systems. As people use these tools, they
bring changes to both the world around them and to the psychological processes
and representations that underlie the activities (Vygotsky, 1978; Martin
& Scribner, 1988).
Technology -- when widely used for children's reading and writing activities
-- is changing the kinds of interactions children have with text, as well
as the kinds of texts with which they interact. New computer-based tools
combine text, sound, image and video in various ways -- as in videodiscs
and video games -- providing new methods for creating texts. In this report,
we see how a multimedia composition tool designed specifically for children
was used by a group of five students in a second grade classroom.
Innovations and Classrooms
Any technology is conceived, developed and employed in a particular social
context, and the influence it has depends greatly upon how it is used in
a given environment. Putting a computer in a classroom is not likely to
lead to changes in learning unless the computer-based activities relate
closely to the kinds of activities already taking place in that classroom.
In Mehan's (1989) study of the use of single microcomputers in four classrooms,
he describes how the technology was used in very different ways by each
teacher, leading to different changes in classroom activity. Mehan argues
that the social structure of the classroom is the key element in understanding
how technology will be used. Although particular uses of technology may
encourage participation, the most important factor is not the computer and
the software, but what is done with them.
Similar conclusions are drawn by Hawkins (1987) in an analysis of Logo use
by teachers and students, by Genishi (1988) who examined the use of Logo
in a kindergarten classroom, and by Cochran-Smith (1991) in a review of
word processing research with children. This research supports the assertion
that technology is interpreted according to the relationships that exist
in the classroom prior to the presence of the technology.
Writing and Technology
Most research on writing with computers has concentrated on cognitive
processes in isolation from particular writing contexts (Cochran-Smith,
Paris & Kahn, 1991), while research on reading and technology has focused
on Computer-Assisted Instruction, such as reading improvement software (Balajthy,
1989). Neither of these approaches seems adequate when attempting to explain
how a particular technology is integrated into the culture of a classroom.
The kinds of changes that may occur are not likely to be captured if the
focus of research is only on individuals and does not attempt to understand
the classroom environment. The manner in which an individual child makes
use of a computer will always be closely connected to the presence of other
children and teachers, for it is the social relationships which exist in
the classroom that will help us understand how and why things happen, whether
or not they involve technology.
As Cochran-Smith et al. (1991) state in their study of word processing and
elementary students:
Learning to write with computers and learning to teach writing with computers are qualitatively different experiences from learning with pencil and paper. (p. 1)
According to Cochran-Smith, word processing can lead to the use of new social
arrangements involving collaboration and coaching, which in turn shape the
theories and practices of writing in the classroom. Although there is little
evidence that the quality of student writing changes when word processors
are available, their study indicates that children may spend more time writing,
and produce texts that are slightly longer than those created using pen
and paper. What is not clear from the study is whether students who have
continuous access to computers use them differently than students who may
use computers once a week in a computer lab, or who have a single computer
available in their classroom.
Literacy
Lemke (1989) has described literacy as knowledge about a world of texts,
and making connections between them, both to understand and to be understood.
Schooling can be seen as learning to master texts deemed important by a
particular society. Barthes (1974) views text as just a set of potential
meanings which are only realized through the reader-text interaction. This
interaction is complex and greatly influenced, as Lemke notes, by other
text with which a reader is familiar. For children accustomed to video games
and television, their intertextual world stretches beyond the book to include
particular characters and stories as well as particular ways of interacting
with texts.
Barthes differentiates between "readerly" and "writerly texts".
The writerly text gives the reader more room to maneuver, and the reader
is more actively involved in creating meaning. With readerly text, in contrast,
the reader is left to accept or reject what is presented, and meaning is
often overly-determined by the writer.
This distribution of power and control between text and reader, and the
role it plays in how meaning is arrived at, seems a key point in understanding
the reading and writing of children who are accustomed to different kinds
of texts, such as video games, which may be seen as very "writerly."
A child interacting with a video game has a large amount of control over
what happens as the game progresses. When the same children create or read
text, similar options may not always be available. When they are, as in
the case of multimedia technologies and computers, children may find the
kinds of interactions possible a more natural extension of video games than
books or other activities available to them.
Computer Supported Cooperative Work
In a review of tools for collaboration, Michael Schrage (1990) makes
a distinction between increasing communication and increasing collaboration,
and emphasizes that different tools are needed for collaboration. Schrage
defines collaboration as shared discovery or shared creation, and shows
that the need for collaboration is great when people deal with complex problems
or when people with different areas of expertise need to work together.
Collaborative tools can help people produce something together that no one
person could produce working alone.
How tools are designed will influence the kinds of collaboration they encourage.
The desk, according to Schrage, is designed for the individual working alone.
The same could be said for most computer software, which usually assumes
one user with one keyboard and one mouse working on one computer. Tools
for collaboration can help people develop what Schrage calls a "shared
space." The shared space can be generated by people separated by time
and distance, but it is through the use of the shared space that a collaboration
will be shaped. Language is the primary way in which this shared understanding
is developed, for as Schrage points out, language is the primary tool for
collaboration.
While not primarily focused on the collaborative aspects of software design,
several studies of word processing among children have considered how the
use of computers for writing encourages collaborative writing. Heap (1989)
points out that collaborative activity makes private cognitive processes
public -- students have to negotiate the use of the computer, and development
of a text, through language. For research purposes, the opportunity to record
and analyze the public thoughts and writing activities of students is an
advantage brought on by collaborative writing. Dickinson's study (1986)
on the use of a computer for writing in a first-second grade classroom focused
on the social structure that arose as the computer was used, and examined
the planning, self-monitoring and response to writing that occurred as a
result. In this case the computer was integrated into the pre-existing writing
curriculum and treated as another tool for writing by the teacher, but its
use led to more opportunities for children to talk among themselves than
was possible when they wrote with pencil and paper. Both Dickinson and Heap
looked at classrooms with one computer, and with word processing software
designed with a single user in mind. In both cases, the cooperative aspects
of composing are defined socially, and are not primary features of the writing
tools in use. Students collaborated because they chose to work together
and because there was a shortage of tools.
Neither of these studies provides a definition of collaborative text. Is
a story written by one child with help from a computer assistant collaborative
in the same way that a story jointly composed by two students is? Certainly
collaboration in writing can exist at a number of levels, but if better
tools to facilitate collaborative composing are to be designed, there needs
to be an understanding of what kinds of collaboration are valuable, and
how software/tool design relates to use in classrooms.
In this report we examine the use of StoryShow, a multimedia composing tool
conceived to help young children construct stories that incorporate images,
and written and oral language. It was designed for use by two students,
although in this case it involved more, with particular features designed
to encourage collaborative composing, editing, and the sharing of tasks.
The primary questions for this study are:
- How does each child get to play the role of author?
- How does the sharing of tasks reflect the social structure already
existing in the classroom?
- How does the intended use of the software differ from how it is actually used in the classroom?
Method
The research method employed in this study is micro-ethnographic. The overall goal of the larger ACOT study, of which this report is one part, is to understand classroom learning activities from the point of view of the participants, to describe how innovative educational tools move from design to actual use, and to show how that knowledge may be applied to future designs.
Setting
This study took place in a second grade classroom in the Silicon Valley
area of California as part of long term research conducted by the Apple
Classrooms of Tomorrow project of Apple Computer, Inc. The second grade
teacher chose to participate in the field testing of StoryShow, which had
previously been in use in the first and fourth grade classrooms of this
ACOT school.
The teacher in this classroom, Ms. Boston, had been teaching for 29 years,
including the previous three years as an ACOT teacher. She had become accustomed
to teaching with technology, and had made some adjustments because of it,
but she was not as eager to use new software and hardware as some other
teachers in the school and preferred to have assistance when trying out
new technologies.
The school population is drawn largely from middle to upper socio-economic
families, with many parents taking a strong interest in their child's schooling.
In Ms. Boston's class there were 27 students, approximately 70 percent Anglo
and 30 percent Asian. The five students participating in this study were
chosen by the teacher, and the activities were conducted during the natural
course of events in the classroom.
The classroom itself was unique in terms of the amount of technology available
to students and teacher: eight Macintosh computers, three scanners, a laser
printer, and a MacRecorder. In addition, there were 16 Apple II GS computers,
eight dot-matrix printers and two video cassette recorders with color monitors.
Another Macintosh, connected to a videodisc player, was shared with another
classroom.
Students used the computers for writing in journals, composing stories or
drawing or creating animation. Computers were not used for playing games
in the classroom. Over the course of a day, students were likely to use
a computer for a total of about one hour.
Reading and writing activities were integrated in Ms. Boston's classroom.
Children often read a particular type of story as a group, an additional
story on their own, then wrote their own story on the same topic. The open
atmosphere in the classroom allowed students to move around relatively freely,
so collaboration of various kinds was likely to take place throughout the
day. Collaboration might involve one student assisting another with a computer
task, or with spelling, or could be a group activity with students reading
aloud or acting out parts of a story.
Description of multimedia composing tool
The primary activity observed and recorded in this study was the use
of the multimedia composing software StoryShow, during one morning session
in Ms. Boston's classroom. The software was developed at Apple Computer,
Inc. for several months before it was introduced in classrooms. StoryShow
was conceived to help children construct stories that incorporate images
and written and oral language. As this study indicates, that is not necessarily
how it will be used in classrooms.
StoryShow is presently designed to run on Macintosh computers with color
monitors, integrating video capture, scanning, sound input, and text. The
video, image scanning, and sound elements are provided through additional
devices attached to the Macintosh -- an 8 mm video camera connected to the
Macintosh using a video capture board, an Apple flatbed scanner for images
in books or drawn by hand on paper, and a MacRecorder for sound input. Each
of these devices can be accessed directly from StoryShow via a single mouse
click. The software uses the metaphor of a slide show, with each slide potentially
consisting of an image, text, and sound. The resulting multimedia text can
be played back on the screen as a series of images, sounds, and text, and
it can be saved to a videotape which can then be replayed on a video cassette
recorder at home or in school.
Initial testing was conducted informally on pairs of six and seven year
old children. This was done early in the design and programming phase of
the project to ensure the program was not too complex for the target audience.
Additional testing continued with first and fourth graders in volunteer
classrooms, and was ongoing during the time of this study.
Data Collection
The data for this study were collected over a five-week period and consisted
of field notes from observations of classroom activity, videotapes of students
working in the classroom and using StoryShow, a video taped trace of what
students produced on the computer screen, the computer files of their work,
and a taped interview with the teacher.
The students in Ms. Boston's class were initially introduced to StoryShow
in a 60 minute demonstration. Several children participated directly, and
the entire class chose pictures and sound that went into the final text.
The following week a group of six children built a multimedia composition
using StoryShow.
The students' next use of StoryShow, and the one reported in this study,
occurred on a day when the classroom was being used as the background for
a local television show about computers and education. Students were assigned
to StoryShow to illustrate the kinds of activities students carried out
with computers. Filming for the television show lasted about 35 minutes,
with some disruptions, and StoryShow activity then continued for an additional
90 minutes.
The student work group consisted initially of two boys, Rick and Donnie,
and two girls, Julie and Amber. A third girl, Mary, joined the group near
the end of the activity. All students in this group are Anglo American with
the exception of Donnie, who is Asian-American.
During data collection I participated as a technical assistant to the children,
correcting problems that came up during their use of the software. I tried
to refrain from providing assistance on content questions entirely, and
kept my technical help to the minimum necessary to allow the children to
use the software.
Data Preparation
The videotapes of children working at the computer and the trace of their
interactions with the software were reviewed and categorized by the events,
participant structures and content produced. I defined an event as bounded
by the start of any activity designed to add a new element to the composition,
and the completion or abandonment of that activity. I focused on who was
operating each of the two main control devices (mouse, microphone), who
provided the content during the given event, and who made the final decision
on content for that event. For content, I considered the use of the three
elements available in each slide -- image, sound, and text.
The goal of this analysis was to understand how the students allocated composing
tasks as they used the software, to understand how the social interactions
that occurred while students used the software fit into the social structure
of the classroom, and to consider the relationship between the design and
intended use of the software, and how it was actually used by students.
Composing Episode
Participant structures were examined in two ways -- software control
(use of the mouse), and content control (who selects what goes into the
composition and who decides what actually gets saved as part of the final
product.) The students were not assigned specific tasks by the teacher,
but the presence of multiple technologies allowed each of the students to
play various roles. In this case, both boys gravitated towards the computer
while one of the girls, Julie, took control of the content.
Donnie controlled the mouse during 37 of the 44 total events I identified
in the composing process, but this technical control did not translate into
control over content or turn taking. Content was dominated by Julie, who
initiated actions that started 18 new events, and decided whether or not
a particular sound or image would be saved for 38 of the 44 events (often
sharing the decision with others.) Julie was recognized by the other students
as the leader of this activity, as they often turned to her when a decision
had to be made. This was consistent with other activities in the classroom,
and Julie's skill as a leader was noted by Ms. Boston both during the time
the group worked with StoryShow and in an interview. During most of the
composing process, Rick maintained control of the microphone, even when
it was not in use. Control of the microphone had little impact on content
or participation, and when she needed it, Julie physically took control
of the microphone from Rick.
The final slide show produced by the group gives evidence of the cooperative
nature of the composing process, but through the presence of multiple authors,
it tends to mask the way in which students arrived at the final text.
Even in the cases where sound or image input was supplied by someone other
than Julie, she often had the final say as to what image was used or what
sound went with the image. For Mary and Rick, Julie provided assistance
in selecting an image and coached them on the exact content of their sounds
by speaking the words herself and having the other child repeat them before
attempting to record the final sound. While this might seem an act of dominating
the content, in each case, Rick and Mary were not quite sure what to say
or what to put in, and Julie's actions could be seen as those of a more
able peer providing the scaffolding necessary for the other children to
successfully complete the task.
The following example illustrates the composing process used by this group.
(In each of this example, I am identified as the "Assistant").
In the first example we see how the students have structured the process
themselves. They have chosen to create a series of slides using books about
rabbits they have read in class.
Composing Example Production of first slide
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
---|---|---|
Donnie: |
Come on get some pictures.Where's the books? | Donnie using mouse |
Julie and Amber hold them in front of the camera | ||
Julie: |
There it is. I want to hold it up (To Amber). | Julie takes book |
Assistant: |
What do you guys want to do? | |
Julie: |
Do the many rabbit stories of Mrs. Boston's class. | Julie sets book down |
Donnie: |
Get closer, get close | |
Julie: |
No, why don't we hold up, why don't we hold up all of the books in front of the camera? | Julie backs away from camera |
Julie: |
(To Rick and Donnie) OK you guys grab a book. | . |
Donnie: |
I'm not, I'm taking pictures. | Donnie still controls mouse |
Julie: |
Grab a book! (emphatically) | Amber gets a book |
Rick: |
And I am making sounds | |
Both boys here show more interest in having control of the hardware than
in actually choosing what goes into the composition. Their language-"I'm taking pictures," and "I am making sounds" imply a link between control of the devices to create the pictures and sounds and the actual production ("making") of them that didn't really exist. (Mrs. Ellis' is the school's computer coordinator). | ||
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
Julie: |
Grab two if you want to. | |
Julie: |
(To Rick and Donnie) Here, take two. | Julie hands them books |
Julie: |
Let's all get in front of the picture. All of these in front. | |
Everyone holds up books. Julie has organized the group to take the picture. | ||
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
Julie: |
Uh oh, I have three | |
Rick: |
Let's call this the introduction. | |
Julie: |
Hey no you guys...(unintelligible) | |
Rick and Donnie are holding their bookjs up and they are blocking everything else. | ||
Julie and Amber: |
Rick! | |
Julie: |
Can you hold this one? | |
Rick: |
It won't get, it won't fit. | |
Julie: |
Well, we all put in | Donnie stands up. |
Julie: |
Donnie, your face is in the way. (laughing) | |
Julie: |
Donnie, put the other one, the Peter Rabbit hook in the front. | |
Donnie: |
How am I gonna take a picture though? | |
Julie: |
Let me see? | |
Assistant: |
Who's gonna click the mouse? | |
Donnie: |
I will | |
Julie: |
Mrs. Ellis. | Julie leaves |
Julie: |
Mrs. Ellis, could you, could you, could you? | Points to computer |
Rick: |
All right, do it | |
Julie: |
Donnie, put up both of them, Mrs. Ellis's gonna do it. | |
Donnie: |
I can hardly do this. | |
Julie: |
Donnie, put up both of them. Put 'em together. | |
David: |
Ah, what's this? This is crazy | David wanders over |
David was not part of the group but happened to walk over to that part of the classroom as they were working. | ||
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
Julie: |
Are they both in the picture? | |
Rick: |
No. | |
Mrs. Ellis: |
There. | Standing next to computer |
Donnie: |
Take a picture | |
Mrs. Ellis: |
Ready? | |
Julie: |
No, wait. Can you see all the books? | |
This is an example of the sort of attention to content and editing that
took place, primarily as initiated by Julie, during the composing process. As they progressed through the composition, more and more time was spent pre-editing, that is, images and sounds were rehearsed or oriented carefully before recording or capturing. | ||
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
Assistant: |
Now click on the face, the face. | |
Mrs. Ellis: |
Where's the mouse? What face, which face? | Couldn't find cursor |
Assistant: |
On the yellow. | |
Donnie: |
This one. (points to yellow icon of a face) | |
Mrs. Ellis: |
OK on the yellow. Got it. | |
The students referred more to the colors of objects on the screen than to
the icons which represented the actions that would be initiated by clicking those objects with the mouse. | ||
Speaker |
What is said | Related Actions |
Mrs. Ellis: |
Ready? Tell me when? | |
Julie: |
No, we don't have all the books in. | |
Mrs. Ellis: |
There...now. Go. | Makes adjustments w/books |
Picture is taken and appears on the screen after about five seconds | ||
Donnie: |
Do you like that? | Using mouse |
All: |
Yeah! |
While composing with StoryShow can be seen clearly as an extension of the
reading and writing activities usually carried out in class -- reading stories
and then writing about them or creating original stories on the same topic
-- it also required that students manage a variety of new roles. Julie initiated
the event by distributing books and positioning the other students in front
of the camera, and it was her idea to show several books in the picture
as a way to capture what this composition was about -- "The Many Rabbit
Stories of Mrs. Boston's Class." However, the successful completion
of the image required the cooperation of each student as well as the assistance
of the school computer coordinator (Mrs. Ellis) who was in the room to help
with the film crew. The process of taking the picture involved selecting
an image, framing it by arranging people and objects in front of the camera,
then capturing the image by clicking the mouse on the appropriate button
on the screen. At this point the captured image freezed momentarily on the
screen, where it could be evaluated by the students. For each slide in this
example, what might be described as editing of a slide took place before
the image was captured. Once an image had been put into the computer, it
was usually saved as is and rarely rejected or retaken. Sounds recorded
for each slide in the show were more often rejected and re-recorded.
It is interesting to note that the way sounds and images were designed to
be processed by the software, paralleled their use by the students. Recorded
sounds were immediately played back by the computer, giving students the
choice of saving or re-recording the sounds. For the final slide in this
example, students recorded the sound six times before saving it. The software
originally worked the same way for sound or images: once recorded or captured,
the sound or image would automatically get saved into a bank of available
sounds and images for later placement. Modification of the sound input facilitated
editing by forcing a choice to save or re-record the sound.
The use of a trial run for sounds seems to have developed as a result of
hesitations which came up during most of the recordings during the first
session. Initially, the sounds were generated on the fly, as they were recorded.
During a second session, Julie began the practice of discussing the sound
first, then practicing it before attempting to record it. The immediate
feedback from the computer seemed to make this editing easier, as children
heard their work right away, and then had the option to save it or record
it again.
Another curious aspect of the use of StoryShow is the gender related differences
in their chosen roles. Donnie and Rick were more interested in having control
of the computer through the mouse and microphone, and seemed less interested
in adding content to the composition. Julie initiated the turn taking that
allowed everyone to have a role in content, and although both Julie and
Amber created two slides each, there was no resistance to this from Rick
or Donnie, who each created one. The same orientation to the composing process
was apparent in other episodes in which these students were involved, with
both boys focused on controlling hardware while Julie had control of content
and orchestration.
Teacher and Technology
In this classroom, as in the first and fourth grade classes, the way StoryShow was used related closely to the social organization of the classroom, especially in the roles students played, their freedom to move around, and ways of working: cooperatively, independently, or for an audience. While Ms. Boston stated,"I'm not terribly comfortable with technology but I've gotten way more so," she also noted that technology, both inside and outside her classroom, had had an effect on the way she taught. At the outset of the ACOT program, Ms. Boston maintained a "lecture-oriented" teaching practice. Students were allowed to become familiar with the computer in a limited way, but it was mainly used to tutor or drill individual students working alone. Over the years, however, she changed her teaching style to better suit her students, who commonly use interactive technologies such as video games and computers, as well as the visually compelling medium of television, outside the classroom. This led her to involve students in more physical ways in the classroom, using dramatization of stories where students act out particular roles within a text, or choral speaking, where students share in the reading of books. In her opinion, a child might tune out sound but respond well to visual information, or vice versa, and providing as many opportunities as possible became important to her. This change in teaching style, Ms. Boston remarked, is in part:
...Because once again I'm in competition with what's going on outside the classroom, so I think I try to change activities so they're involved. Without technology, they're involved doing dramatization or in choral speaking or in coming up and doing an example. But physically they're involved because I think physically they're involved in a lot of things outside the classroom.
An additional change was that the computer was no longer studied for knowledge
of its components or functions. Ms. Boston's students were already tutored
in computer basics in the first grade, so most were already familiar with
the keyboard and had begun to master touch typing.
Independent work in Ms. Boston's classroom was common. They wrote daily
journals on the computer, and they also helped each other by proofreading
and offering suggestions. Students also worked in small groups on computer-oriented
tasks and they helped each other in both technology and non-technology activities.
Ms. Boston used a process-oriented approach to writing, with a chart on
the wall listing various steps in the writing process as a guideline for
children -- pre-writing, writing, revision, etc. In this classroom, the
process of revision seemed to match Cochran-Smith (1991) -- students corrected
errors and added to the end of texts, but were less likely to move text
around or change parts of sentences. Ms. Boston said it was "important
that children write every day," and student work on journals and in
writing their own stories was something that went on every day in her classroom.
Most of the computer use involved children using painting or writing programs
at individual computers, with various kinds of helping and sharing of information,
both about content and about how to use the computer.
One of the original goals of the design of StoryShow was to build a composing
tool that could be easily used by two students working together on the same
project. Choosing projects and students was left to the teachers involved
in this study, and one result was that different kinds of classrooms and
teaching approaches led to different kinds of collaborations among students
using StoryShow.
In Ms. Boston's class, all student editing was at the point of input. Features
in the software designed to enable the movement of slides and slide elements
were not used at all by this group. These features occupied large amounts
of development time, and were included to make possible the sorts of higher-level
editing paralleled by the copy, cut, and paste features of word processors.
Perhaps not surprisingly, although this group of students has highly developed
skills for using the computer, their editing/revision strategies tended
to be surface level only. Even in writing with word processors they often
made spelling corrections or added text to existing paragraphs or corrected
sentences as they wrote them, but didn't move things around, or attempt
to completely rewrite existing sentences. Ms. Boston said that students
did do more revision of their work with the computer, but most of that activity
centered around correcting spelling errors. As a result, it is not surprising
that the editing features built in to StoryShow were not used. Again, classroom
practice was the primary factor in how the technology was actually used.
A useful question to consider is how existing practice might be successfully
combined with technology to encourage particular types of writing and composing
activities that are seen as desirable, such as higher-level editing and
revision. Attempting to encourage editing by providing tools to facilitate
it is inadequate -- the tasks students engage in need to require that editing
be a desirable part of the process.
While existing classroom activities did not encourage much editing or revision
of story elements, the classroom structure did seem to support the sort
of independent, student-centered group work that the StoryShow activity
permits, and students' experience with computers as well as working with
each other seemed to facilitate the group production of text. They were
accustomed to sharing tasks in large group productions such as choral readings,
and they had experience writing and sharing stories with the rest of the
class. In the other two classrooms, students worked only in pairs, and where
the pairs were accustomed to working together things went relatively smoothly,
but when they were unfamiliar with each other's styles, the process was
not as smooth. A larger group makes this kind of individual conflict less
likely, although it also complicates the distribution of power in the composing
process, as this report shows.
Overall, the presence of an integrated reading-writing program where students
were accustomed to writing about things they read and sharing that with
other students, as well as working together, seemed to facilitate the use
of StoryShow in interesting ways. This activity was seen as valuable by
the students involved, in part because they were doing something that other
students were not, but also because their work was presented to the whole
class with the lights out. Knowing that an audience existed for their composition
seems to have played a role both in the structure of their composition --
an introductory slide announcing the content and a final slide identifying
each author -- as well as in their emphasis on getting each sound right
and making sure it was clearly audible. Their final slide took six tries
before the sound was deemed acceptable by the group.
While StoryShow was designed for two children working together, it really
doesn't provide any special tools to enhance the collaboration. Language,
as Schrage (1990) noted, is ultimately the primary tool of collaboration.
Certainly language is the medium through which students in this study shared
power and content. Control of the technology was only a secondary feature
of the collaborative process. Language was used to direct the content and
the operation of the software, to accept or reject images and sounds that
became part of the composition, and to plan each slide as it was constructed.
While the software can be seen as the focus of the process, language was
really the tool of collaboration.
Software Design
Although students had only limited experience using the software, they
were able to manage the process of importing images from the video camera
and record sounds without much difficulty. To a large extent, the software
did not get in the way of their activity. However, there were several instances
where moving from one "mode" to another was confusing. For example,
they employed a consistently linear process when constructing their composition
-- they moved from slide #1 through to slide #9 one by one, first capturing
an image and then adding a sound to the image. No text was used for any
slides. Breakdowns, defined as instances where production or manipulation
of an element was interrupted by a problem with the software, seemed to
occur when students moved from image capture to sound recording, a procedure
which involved clicking on specific buttons to close the video camera and
return to the slide editing area. On several occasions students opened a
new window to capture an image on one monitor, then moved back to the main
slide window without closing the capture window, causing the software to
function incorrectly. In addition, this movement necessitated the dragging
of a screen window from one monitor to another each time. This process is
initially very disorienting, as it involves moving an object between two
screens which are physically separate. When the object is dragged to the
right edge of one screen, it then appears on the left edge of the second
screen, which is several inches away from the first screen. Use of the second
screen was necessary to allow videotaping of screen interactions as well
as the use of the video capture for images. A more elegant solution might
be to incorporate the image capture directly into each individual slide
to avoid the mode switching and perhaps facilitating faster construction
of the compositions.
Because these children did no editing other than to reject particular images
or sounds and retake or re-record them, the switching from capture to edit
mode served no purpose other than to confuse them.
A second feature of the software which was not used at all again relates
to the modality of the slide images. An initial screen shows 15 slides in
small form images are approximately 1/20 original size, and text and sounds
appear only as icons on individual slides.
To view or hear the slide, one can double click the mouse on the image or
part of the slide. In this study, that operation was not performed even
once. Editing decisions were made at the point of import -- when an image
was grabbed using the camera and video capture software/hardware, it was
either accepted or rejected. The same procedure was followed for sounds.
Again, redesign of the software could make this process easier by eliminating
the use of the small slides and filling each screen with the original size
image along with the image capture controls, sound controls and text area.
Image, not text, predominates in the design of the software, so a redesign
that incorporates text more prominently might increase the use of written
language by younger children.
With most software, files are saved with names given by the user of the
software. Instead, StoryShow generates names for each image, sound, and
text, and each of these objects are kept as small icons at the bottom of
the screen to be added to a specific slide by dragging the image up to the
slide. For images and text, this works well, as the icon usually provides
a visible clue as to what the entire image or text is -- a shrunken picture
or the first few words of the text. For sounds, the visual representation
offers no clue as to the content, making browsing through sounds a tedious
process of listening to each one entirely. What is needed is a way to sample
the first two or three seconds of the sound. Lacking this, the size of the
sound is represented by icons which show more waves coming out of a speaker
to indicate longer sounds.
This feature, and others with a "collect-browse-assemble" orientation
tended to be ignored by students. Instead, all composing was done one slide
at a time. A picture was captured and saved, followed by a sound. This eliminated
the need to browse through images and sounds, and argues for a redesign
of the software to better fit the composition style of the students.
Collaboration and StoryShow
As a collaborative tool, StoryShow functions reasonably well in an environment
where collaboration is already common. The presence of multiple input devices,
and multiple roles for participants, allow groups of up to five or six children
to work together on a composition. What is clear is that the collaboration
is tied to the social setting in which the software is used and is not an
outgrowth of any particular design features aimed at encouraging collaboration.
In a first grade classroom where students were less accustomed to working
independently and were paired as a result of completing an assignment first
rather than by choice, the software was used one at a time, with each child
creating a single slide as the other watched or assisted with reading of
the text, or correcting spelling errors on the screen. This kind of collaboration
is similar to that among the first graders using word processing software
described by Heap (1989). When one student has final control over content,
whether for the text of a story or for the image, sound, and text in an
individual slide, it is not clear that another student assisting with input
or corrections is really collaborating as a coauthor. To call such interactions
collaboration may honor the work of the assistant too highly. Collaboration
at the level of content seems a different matter than sharing the task of
typing. In Heap's study and others like it, collaboration is part of the
social structure of the classroom that permits it to happen, but it often
occurs because there is a limited amount of technology, often one computer
per classroom. Such a set-up almost requires that children work together
if enough students are to master the computer, on whatever software is available.
Designing software specifically for collaborative work will require attention
both to the writing practices of classrooms as well as an understanding
of the kinds of social relationships that exist among students and teachers
in classrooms.
This report was part of a larger study of the introduction of multimedia
software designed for use by pairs of children. In each of the three classrooms
where the study was conducted, students used StoryShow to construct presentations
rather than stories, and in Ms. Boston's classroom, students worked in groups
of four to six rather than in pairs. A number of factors contributed to
how the software ultimately was used, including how it was interpreted by
individual teachers, how well it fit into the ongoing reading and writing
activities of the classroom, and how students chose to use it. While the
software design can be seen as successful from a technical perspective because
students in first, second, and fourth grades were all able to use it with
very limited instruction, a closer look at actual use suggests that technical
mastery is not closely tied to using software in ways that are interesting
or involve children in reading and writing in new ways.
The presence of advanced technology for creating multimedia compositions
requires curriculum support that gives students a sense of how to combine
different media effectively. Given the extremely limited presence of multimedia
composing tools in schools, it is unlikely to expect teachers to easily
integrate such tools into classrooms which still tend to focus on paper
as the primary medium of communication, whether as part of writing, drawing,
or word processing activities.
Software designed for particular audiences and tasks will be used in ways
that are supported by the context rather than by the software. In this case,
what mattered most was not the presence of expensive computer hardware and
the software to allow students to create multimedia compositions, but a
classroom environment which supported the kinds of independent and collaborative
activities that the software made possible. Without a supportive environment,
the software was little more than an object of fascination for bringing
together a variety of new technologies, and permitting students to replicate
their work in a new medium.
Discussion
As Mehan (1989), Cochran-Smith (1991), Hawkins (1987) and Genishi (1988) have shown, technology is not always the key element when considering how a particular hardware or software innovation actually gets used by students. The classroom is a social environment with a variety of relationships, norms, and practices which exist prior to the introduction of a new technology. How a new technology is used owes as much to the ongoing activities of the classroom as it does to any particular features of the technology.
References
Balajthy, E. (1989) Computers and Reading, Prentice-Hall, Englewood
Cliffs, NJ.
Barthes, R. (1974) S/Z (Richard Miller, Trans.). Hill and Wang, New
York.
Cochran-Smith, M. (1991) Word Processing and Writing in Elementary
Classrooms: A Critical Review of Related Literature, Review of Educational
Research, 61(1), 107-55.
Cochran-Smith, M., Paris, C. L., & Kahn, J. (1991) Learning
to Write Differently: Beginning Writers and Word Processing, Ablex,
Norwood, NJ.
Dickinson, D. K. (1986) Cooperation, Collaboration, and a Computer:
Integrating a Computer into a First-Second Grade Writing Program, Research
in the Teaching of English, 20(4), 357-378.
Dwyer, D., and Reilly, B. (1990) StoryShow, (computer software) Apple
Computer, Inc.
Genishi, C. (1988) Kindergartners and computers: A case study of
six children. The Elementary School Journal, 89, 184-201.
Hawkins, J. (1987) The Interpretation of Logo in Practice, in R.
D. Pea & K. Sheingold (Eds.), Mirrors of Minds - Patterns of Experience
in Educational Computing 3-34. Ablex, Norwood, NJ.
Heap, J. L. (1989) Collaborative Practices during Word Processing
in a First Grade Classroom. In C. Emihovich (Eds.), Locating Learning:
Ethnographic Perspectives on Classroom Research, Ablex, Norwood, NJ.
Heap, J. L. (1989) Sociality and Cognition in Collaborative Computer
Writing, in D. Bloome (Eds.), Classrooms and Literacy, Ablex, Norwood,
NJ.
Lemke, J. L. (1989) Social Semiotics: A New Model for Literacy Education,
in D. Bloome (Ed.), Classrooms and Literacy 289-309. Ablex, Norwood,
NJ.
Martin, L.W. & Scribner, S. (1988) An Introduction to CNC
Systems: Background for Learning and Training Research. Laboratory for Cognitive
Studies of Work, New York.
Mehan, H. (1989) Microcomputers in classrooms: Educational technology
or social practice? Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 20, 4-22.
Schrage, Michael (1990) Shared Minds: The new technologies of
collaboration, Random House, New York.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978) Mind in Society. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Note: StoryShow, the multimedia software used in this study, was developed
at Apple Computer, Inc. by the author, Brian Reilly, based on an original
concept by David C. Dwyer Ph.D., project manager and principal scientist
of Apple Classrooms of Tomorrow.
Note: All names in this report are pseudonyms.
Editors for this report were Jenny Abbe and Mary A. C. Fallon.